Showing 1 - 10 of 420
Game theory is usually difficult to test precisely in the field because predictions typically depend sensitively on features that are not controlled or observed. We conduct one such test using field data from the Swedish lowest unique positive integer (LUPI) game. In the LUPI game, players pick...
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10004961389
Game theory is usually difficult to test precisely in the field because predictions typically depend sensitively on features that are not controlled or observed. We conduct one such test using field data from the Swedish lowest unique positive integer (LUPI) game. In the LUPI game, players pick...
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10003516443
Game theory is usually difficult to test in the field because predictions typically depend sensitively on features that are not controlled or observed. We conduct one such test using both laboratory and field data from the Swedish lowest unique positive integer (LUPI) game. In this game, players...
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10009216726
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10009904858
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10009271676
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10011432242
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10012239508
We report results from an exploratory study using eye-tracking recording of information acquisition by players in a game theoretic learning paradigm. Eye-tracking is used to observe what information subjects look at in 4 × 4 normal-form games; the eye-tracking results favor sophisticated...
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10004992791
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10005828027
We report experiments on sender-receiver games with an incentive for senders to exaggerate. Subjects "overcommunicate" -- messages are more informative of the true state than they should be, in equilibrium. Eyetracking shows that senders look at payoffs in a way that is consistent with a level-k...
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10008542945